Color and Psychological Functioning a Review of Theoretical and Empirical Work

The past decade has seen enhanced involvement in inquiry in the area of color and psychological functioning. Progress has been fabricated on both theoretical and empirical fronts, only there are also weaknesses on both of these fronts that must be attended to for this research area to go along to make progress. In the following, I briefly review both advances and weaknesses in the literature on color and psychological functioning.

Theoretical Work

Groundwork and Recent Developments

Color has fascinated scholars for millennia (Sloane, 1991; Gage, 1993). Theorizing on color and psychological performance has been present since Goethe (1810) penned his Theory of Colors, in which he linked colour categories (e.g., the "plus" colors of yellowish, red–yellow, yellowish–cherry) to emotional responding (e.g., warmth, excitement). Goldstein (1942) expanded on Goethe'southward intuitions, positing that certain colors (e.g., red, yellow) produce systematic physiological reactions manifest in emotional feel (eastward.yard., negative arousal), cognitive orientation (due east.chiliad., outward focus), and overt activity (e.g., forceful behavior). Subsequent theorizing derived from Goldstein's ideas has focused on wavelength, positing that longer wavelength colors experience arousing or warm, whereas shorter wavelength colors feel relaxing or absurd (Nakashian, 1964; Crowley, 1993). Other conceptual statements about colour and psychological functioning have focused on general associations that people take to colors and their corresponding influence on downstream affect, noesis, and behavior (eastward.k., black is associated with aggression and elicits aggressive beliefs; Frank and Gilovich, 1988; Soldat et al., 1997). Finally, much writing on color and psychological functioning has been completely atheoretical, focused exclusively on finding answers to applied questions (eastward.g., "What wall colour facilitates worker alertness and productivity?"). The same theories and conceptual statements continue to motivate inquiry on color and psychological functioning. However, several other promising theoretical frameworks have likewise emerged in the past decade, and I review these frameworks in the following.

Hill and Barton (2005) noted that in many non-human animals, including primate species, dominance in aggressive encounters (i.e., superior concrete condition) is signaled by the brilliant blood-red of oxygenated claret visible on highly vascularized bare pare. Bogus ruddy (e.chiliad., on leg bands) has besides been shown to signal authorisation in not-human animals, mimicking the natural physiological procedure (Cuthill et al., 1997). In humans in aggressive encounters, a testosterone surge produces visible reddening on the face and fear leads to pallor (Drummond and Quay, 2001; Levenson, 2003). Hill and Barton (2005) posited that the parallel between humans and non-humans present at the physiological level may extend to artificial stimuli, such that wearing ruby in sport contests may convey dominance and lead to a competitive advantage.

Other theorists have besides utilized a comparative approach in positing links between skin coloration and the evaluation of conspecifics. Changizi et al. (2006) and Changizi (2009) contend that trichromatic vision evolved to enable primates, including humans, to discover subtle changes in blood catamenia beneath the pare that carry important data about the emotional state of the conspecific. Increased ruby can convey acrimony, embarrassment, or sexual arousal, whereas increased blue or light-green tint can convey illness or poor physiological condition. Thus, visual sensitivity to these color modulations facilitates diverse forms of social interaction. In similar fashion, Stephen et al. (2009) and Stephen and McKeegan (2010) propose that perceivers use information about skin coloration (perhaps specially from the face, Tan and Stephen, 2012) to make inferences about the attractiveness, health, and dominance of conspecifics. Redness (from claret oxygenization) and yellowness (from carotenoids) are both seen as facilitating positive judgments. Fink et al. (2006) and Fink and Matts (2007) posit that the homogeneity of skin coloration is an of import factor in evaluating the historic period, bewitchery, and health of faces.

Elliot and Maier (2012) have proposed color-in-context theory, which draws on social learning, as well every bit biology. Some responses to color stimuli are presumed to exist solely due to the repeated pairing of colour and particular concepts, messages, and experiences. Others, yet, are presumed to represent a biologically engrained predisposition that is reinforced and shaped by social learning. Through this social learning, color associations can be extended across natural actual processes (e.g., blood menstruum modulations) to objects in shut proximity to the body (east.g., dress, accessories). Thus, for example, red may not simply increment attractiveness evaluations when viewed on the face, merely also when viewed on a shirt or dress. As implied by the name of the theory, the concrete and psychological context in which color is perceived is thought to influence its significant and, accordingly, responses to information technology. Thus, blue on a ribbon is positive (indicating first place), only blue on a piece of meat is negative (indicating rotten), and a cherry shirt may enhance the attractiveness of a potential mate (red = sex/romance), merely non of a person evaluating one'due south competence (red = failure/danger).

Meier and Robinson (2005) and Meier (in press) have posited a conceptual metaphor theory of color. From this perspective, people talk and recollect nearly abstract concepts in concrete terms grounded in perceptual experience (i.eastward., they use metaphors) to help them understand and navigate their social world (Lakoff and Johnson, 1999). Thus, acrimony entails reddening of the confront, so anger is metaphorically described as "seeing red," and positive emotions and experiences are often depicted in terms of lightness (rather than darkness), so lightness is metaphorically linked to good ("seeing the light") rather than bad ("in the night"). These metaphoric associations are presumed to have implications for important outcomes such as morality judgments (e.g., white things are viewed equally pure) and stereotyping (e.chiliad., dark faces are viewed more negatively).

For many years information technology has been known that lite directly influences physiology and increases arousal (see Cajochen, 2007, for a review), but recently theorists have posited that such effects are wavelength dependent. Bluish lite, in particular, is posited to activate the melanopsin photoreceptor system which, in turn, activates the encephalon structures involved in sub-cortical arousal and higher-order attentional processing (Cajochen et al., 2005; Lockley et al., 2006). As such, exposure to blue light is expected to facilitate alacrity and enhance performance on tasks requiring sustained attention.

Evaluation and Recommendations

Drawing on recent theorizing in evolutionary psychology, emotion scientific discipline, retinal physiology, person perception, and social cognition, the same conceptualizations represent important advances to the literature on colour and psychological functioning. Nonetheless, theory in this surface area remains at a nascent level of development, and the following weaknesses may exist identified.

First, the focus of theoretical work in this surface area is either extremely specific or extremely general. A precise conceptual proposition such as red signals say-so and leads to competitive advantage in sports (Colina and Barton, 2005) is valuable in that information technology can be directly translated into a clear, testable hypothesis; nevertheless, it is non clear how this specific hypothesis connects to a broader understanding of colour–performance relations in achievement settings more than by and large. On the other finish of the spectrum, a general conceptualization such every bit color-in-context theory (Elliot and Maier, 2012) is valuable in that it offers several widely applicable premises; however, these premises are only vaguely suggestive of precise hypotheses in specific contexts. What is needed are mid-level theoretical frameworks that comprehensively, yet precisely explicate and predict links betwixt color and psychological functioning in specific contexts (for emerging developments, see Pazda and Greitemeyer, in printing; Spence, in press; Stephen and Perrett, in press).

Second, the extant theoretical work is limited in telescopic in terms of range of hues, range of colour properties, and direction of influence. Most theorizing has focused on 1 hue, red, which is understandable given its prominence in nature, on the body, and in gild (Changizi, 2009; Elliot and Maier, 2014); yet, other hues also carry important associations that undoubtedly have downstream effects (e.thou., blue: Labrecque and Milne, 2012; green: Akers et al., 2012). Color has three bones properties: hue, lightness, and chroma (Fairchild, 2013). Variation in any or all of these properties could influence downstream affect, cognition, or behavior, yet but hue is considered in well-nigh theorizing (most likely because experientially, it is the nigh salient color belongings). Lightness and chroma also undoubtedly have implications for psychological functioning (e.thou., lightness: Kareklas et al., 2014; chroma: Lee et al., 2013); lightness has received some attention within conceptual metaphor theory (Meier, in press; see as well Prado-León and Rosales-Cinco, 2011), but chroma has been almost entirely overlooked, as has the issue of combinations of hue, lightness, and chroma. Finally, virtually theorizing has focused on color every bit an independent variable rather than a dependent variable; however, it is as well likely that many situational and intrapersonal factors influence color perception (e.thou., situational: Bubl et al., 2009; intrapersonal: Fetterman et al., 2015).

Third, theorizing to date has focused primarily on master furnishings, with merely a modicum of attending allocated to the important issue of moderation. As inquiry literatures develop and mature, they progress from a sole focus on "is" questions ("Does 10 influence Y?") to additionally considering "when" questions ("Under what conditions does X influence Y and nether what conditions does X non influence Y?"). These "2d generation" questions (Zanna and Fazio, 1982, p. 283) can seem less heady and even deflating in that they posit boundary weather that constrain the generalizability of an effect. Nevertheless, this stride is invaluable in that information technology adds conceptual precision and clarity, and begins to address the effect of real-world applicability. All color furnishings undoubtedly depend on certain conditions – culture, gender, historic period, type of task, variant of colour, etc. – and acquiring an understanding of these conditions will represent an important marker of maturity for this literature (for movement in this direction, see Schwarz and Vocalizer, 2013; Tracy and Beall, 2014; Bertrams et al., 2015; Buechner et al., in press; Young, in printing). Another, more succinct, fashion to land this third weakness is that theorizing in this area needs to take context, in all its forms, more seriously.

Empirical Piece of work

Background and Contempo Developments

Empirical work on color and psychological functioning dates back to the late 19th century (Féré, 1887; see Pressey, 1921, for a review). A consequent feature of this work, from its inception to the past decade, is that it has been fraught with major methodological problems that have precluded rigorous testing and articulate interpretation (O'Connor, 2011). Ane problem has been a failure to nourish to rudimentary scientific procedures such as experimenter blindness to condition, identifying, and excluding colour deficient participants, and standardizing the duration of colour presentation or exposure. Some other problem has been a failure to specify and control for color at the spectral level in manipulations. Without such specification, it is impossible to know what precise combination of colour properties was investigated, and without such control, the confounding of focal and not-focal color properties is inevitable (Whitfield and Wiltshire, 1990; Valdez and Mehrabian, 1994). Even so some other trouble has been the use of underpowered samples. This problem, shared across scientific disciplines (Maxwell, 2004), can lead to Type I errors, Blazon Two errors, and inflated effect sizes (Fraley and Vazire, 2014; Murayama et al., 2014). Together, these methodological problems have greatly hampered progress in this area.

Although some of the aforementioned problems remain (see "Evaluation and Recommendations" below), others have been rectified in contempo work. This, coupled with advances in theory development, has led to a surge in empirical activeness. In the following, I review the diverse areas in which color work has been conducted in the past decade, and the findings that have emerged. Space considerations require me to constrain this review to a brief mention of central findings inside each area. I focus on findings with humans (for reviews of research with non-human animals, see Higham and Winters, in press; Setchell, in press) that have been obtained in multiple (at least five) independent labs. Tabular array 1 provides a summary, likewise as representative examples and specific references.

www.frontiersin.org

TABLE i. Research on color and psychological functioning.

In research on colour and selective attention, red stimuli have been shown to receive an attentional reward (see Folk, in printing, for a review). Research on color and alacrity has shown that blueish lite increases subjective alertness and performance on attention-based tasks (run into Chellappa et al., 2011, for a review). Studies on colour and athletic functioning accept linked wearing red to better performance and perceived operation in sport competitions and tasks (encounter Maier et al., in press, for a review). In enquiry on color and intellectual performance, viewing crimson prior to a challenging cognitive task has been shown to undermine performance (come across Shi et al., 2015, for a review). Research focused on color and aggressiveness/dominance evaluation has shown that viewing cherry-red on self or other increases appraisals of aggressiveness and dominance (see Krenn, 2014, for a review). Empirical work on colour and avoidance motivation has linked viewing red in achievement contexts to increased caution and avoidance (see Elliot and Maier, 2014, for a review). In enquiry on colour and attraction, viewing red on or near a female has been shown to heighten attraction in heterosexual males (see Pazda and Greitemeyer, in press, for a review). Research on color and store/company evaluation has shown that blue on stores/logos increases quality and trustworthiness appraisals (encounter Labrecque and Milne, 2012, for a review). Finally, empirical piece of work on color and eating/drinking has shown that red influences food and beverage perception and consumption (run into Spence, in press, for a review).

Evaluation and Recommendations

The aforementioned findings represent important contributions to the literature on color and psychological functioning, and highlight the multidisciplinary nature of research in this area. Nevertheless, much similar the extant theoretical work, the extant empirical work remains at a nascent level of development, due, in role, to the post-obit weaknesses.

Get-go, although in some research in this expanse color properties are controlled for at the spectral level, in well-nigh research it (however) is not. Colour control is typically washed improperly at the device (rather than the spectral) level, is impossible to implement (e.g., in web-based platform studies), or is ignored altogether. Color command is admittedly difficult, as it requires technical equipment for colour assessment and presentation, as well as the expertise to use it. Still, careful color command is essential if systematic scientific work is to be conducted in this expanse. Findings from uncontrolled research can be informative in initial explorations of color hypotheses, but such work is inherently fraught with interpretational ambivalence (Whitfield and Wiltshire, 1990; Elliot and Maier, 2014) that must be after addressed.

Second, color perception is non only a function of lightness, chroma, and hue, simply also of factors such equally viewing distance and angle, amount and type of ambient light, and presence of other colors in the firsthand groundwork and general environmental surround (Hunt and Arrow, 2011; Brainard and Radonjić, 2014; Fairchild, 2015). In basic colour science research (due east.thou., on colour physics, color physiology, colour appearance modeling, etcetera; see Gegenfurtner and Ennis, in press; Johnson, in press; Stockman and Brainard, in press), these factors are carefully specified and controlled for in order to establish standardized participant viewing conditions. These factors have been largely ignored and immune to vary in research on colour and psychological functioning, with unknown consequences. An important next step for research in this area is to move to incorporate these more rigorous standardization procedures widely utilized by basic colour scientists. With regard to both this and the aforementioned weakness, it should be acknowledged that exact and complete control is not actually possible in color enquiry, given the multitude of factors that influence colour perception (Commission on Colorimetry of the Optical Society of America, 1953) and our electric current level of knowledge nearly and ability to control them (Fairchild, 2015). Equally such, the standard that must be embraced and used equally a guideline in this work is to control color backdrop and viewing conditions to the extent possible given electric current technology, and to continue upward with advances in the field that will increasingly afford more precise and efficient color management.

Third, although in some inquiry in this area, large, fully powered samples are used, much of the research remains underpowered. This is a problem in general, but it is particularly a problem when the initial demonstration of an upshot is underpowered (e.one thousand., Elliot and Niesta, 2008), because initial work is often used as a guide for determining sample size in subsequent work (both heuristically and via power analysis). Underpowered samples commonly produce overestimated effect size estimates (Ioannidis, 2008), and basing subsequent sample sizes on such estimates simply perpetuates the problem. Small sample sizes can too lead researchers to prematurely conclude that a hypothesis is disconfirmed, overlooking a potentially of import accelerate (Murayama et al., 2014). Findings from small-scale sampled studies should be considered preliminary; running large sampled studies with advisedly controlled color stimuli is essential if a robust scientific literature is to be adult. Furthermore, every bit the "evidentiary value movement" (Finkel et al., 2015) makes inroads in the empirical sciences, color scientists would do well to be at the leading edge of implementing such rigorous practices as publically archiving research materials and data, designating exploratory from confirmatory analyses, supplementing or fifty-fifty replacing significant testing with "new statistics" (Cumming, 2014), and fifty-fifty preregistering research protocols and analyses (see Finkel et al., 2015, for an overview).

Determination

In both reviewing advances in and identifying weaknesses of the literature on colour and psychological performance, information technology is important to carry in mind that the existing theoretical and empirical piece of work is at an early stage of evolution. Information technology is premature to offer any bold theoretical statements, definitive empirical pronouncements, or impassioned calls for awarding; rather, information technology is best to be patient and to humbly acknowledge that color psychology is a uniquely complex area of inquiry (Kuehni, 2012; Fairchild, 2013) that is only starting time to come into its own. Findings from colour research can be provocative and media friendly, and the public (and the field as well) tin be tempted to attain conclusions before the science is fully in place. There is considerable promise in enquiry on colour and psychological functioning, merely considerably more theoretical and empirical work needs to be done before the full extent of this promise can be discerned and, hopefully, fulfilled.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential disharmonize of interest.

References

Aiken, Thou. D., and Pascal, V. J. (2013). Seeing cherry, feeling red: how a change in field color influences perceptions. Int. J. Sport Soc. three, 107–120.

Google Scholar

Akers, A., Barton, J., Cossey, R., Gainsford, P., Griffin, Chiliad., and Micklewright, D. (2012). Visual color perception in greenish do: positive effects of mood on perceived exertion. Environ. Sci. Technol. 46, 8661–8666. doi: ten.1021/es301685g

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alberts, W., and van der Geest, T. M. (2011). Color matters: color every bit trustworthiness cue in websites. Tech. Comm. 58, 149–160.

Google Scholar

Barli,Ö., Bilgili, B., and Dane, Ş. (2006). Association of consumers' sex and eyedness and lighting and wall color of a store with toll allure and perceived quality of appurtenances and inside visual appeal. Percept. Motor Skill 103, 447–450. doi: 10.2466/PMS.103.half-dozen.447-450

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bertrams, A., Baumeister, R. F., Englert, C., and Furley, P. (2015). Ego depletion in color priming research: self-control strength moderates the detrimental issue of cherry-red on cerebral test performance. Pers. Soc. Psychol. B. 41, 311–322. doi: 10.1177/0146167214564968

PubMed Abstruse | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brainard, D. H., and Radonjić, A. (2014). "Color constancy" in The New Visual Neurosciences, eds J. Werner and L. Chalupa (Cambridge, MA; MIT Printing), 545–556.

Google Scholar

Bruno, Due north., Martani, M., Corsini, C., and Oleari, C. (2013). The consequence of the color red on consuming food does non depend on achromatic (Michelson) contrast and extends to rubbing cream on the skin. Ambition 71, 307–313. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.08.012

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bubl, Due east., Kern, E., Ebert, D., Bach, M., and Tebartz van Elst, L. (2009). Seeing gray when feeling blue? Depression can exist measures in the eye of the diseased. Biol. Psychiat. 68, 205–208. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.02.009

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buechner, 5. 50., Maier, M. A., Lichtenfeld, S., and Elliot, A. J. (in printing). Emotion expression and color: their articulation influence on perceptions of male attractiveness and social position. Curr. Psychol.

Google Scholar

Cajochen, C., Frey, S., Anders, D., Späti, J., Bues, Chiliad., Pross, A., et al. (2011). Evening exposure to a low-cal-emitting diodes (LED)-backlit computer screen affects circadian physiology and cognitive functioning. J. Appl. Phsysoil. 110, 1432–1438. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00165.2011

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cajochen, C., Münch, M., Kobialka, South., Kräuchi, K., Steiner, R., Oelhafen, P., et al. (2005). High sensitivity of homo melatonin, alacrity, thermoregulation, and center charge per unit to brusk wavelength light. J. Clin. Endocr. Metab. 90, 1311–1316. doi: 10.1210/jc.2004-0957

PubMed Abstract | Total Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caldwell, D. F., and Burger, J. M. (2011). On sparse ice: does uniform colour really affect aggression in professional hockey? Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 2, 306–310. doi: 10.1177/1948550610389824

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Changizi, M. (2009). The Vision Revolution. Dallas, TX: Benbella.

Google Scholar

Chebat, J. C., and Morrin, M. (2007). Colors and cultures: exploring the furnishings of mall décor on consumer perceptions. J. Autobus. Res. threescore, 189–196. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2006.11.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chellappa, S. L., Steiner, R., Blattner, P., Oelhafen, P., Götz, T., and Cajochen, C. (2011). Non-visual furnishings of lite on melatonin, alertness, and cerebral performance: tin blue-enriched lite keep u.s. alarm? PLoS 1 26:e16429. doi: 10.1371/periodical.pone.0016429

PubMed Abstruse | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Commission on Colorimetry of the Optical Guild of America. (1953). The Science of Color. Washington, DC: Optical Society of America.

Google Scholar

Cuthill, I. C., Hunt, S., Cleary, C., and Clark, C. (1997). Color bands, authority, and trunk mass regulation in male zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata). Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. Sci. 264, 1093–1099. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1997.0151

CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar

Elliot, A. J., and Maier, Chiliad. A. (2012). Colour-in-context theory. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 45, 61–125. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-394286-9.00002-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elliot, A. J., Maier, Yard. A., Moller, A. C., Friedman, R., and Meinhardt, J. (2007). Color and psychological functioning: the effect of cerise on performance attainment. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 136, 154–168. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.136.one.154

PubMed Abstruse | Full Text | CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar

Elwood, J. A., and Bode, J. (2014). Student preferences vis-à-vis instructor feedback in university EFL writing classes in Japan. Organisation 42, 333–343. doi: x.1016/j.organization.2013.12.023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fairchild, Thou. D. (2015). Seeing, adapting to, and reproducing the appearance of nature. Appl. Optics 54, B107–B116. doi: ten.1364/AO.54.00B107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feltman, R., and Elliot, A. J. (2011). The influence of reddish on perceptions of authorisation and threat in a competitive context. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 33, 308–314.

PubMed Abstruse | Total Text | Google Scholar

Fetterman, A. K., Liu, T., and Robinson, K. D. (2015). Extending color psychology to the personality realm: interpersonal hostility varied by red preferences and perceptual biases. J. Personal. 83, 106–116. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12087

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar

Féré, C. (1887). Annotation sur les conditions physiologiques des émotions. Revue Phil. 24, 561–581.

Google Scholar

Fink, B., Grammar, Chiliad., and Matts, P. J. (2006). Visible pare color distribution plays a role in the perception of age, attractiveness, and wellness in female faces. Evol. Hum. Behav. 27, 433–442. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.08.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finkel, E. J., Eastwick, P. West., and Reis, H. T. (2015). Best research practices in psychology: Illustrating epistemological and pragmatic considerations with the example of relationship science. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 108, 275–297. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000007

PubMed Abstract | Total Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folk, C. L. (in press). "The part of color in the voluntary and involuntary guidance of selective attending," in Handbook of Color Psychology, eds A. Elliot, Chiliad. Fairchild, and A. Franklin (Cambridge: Cambridge Academy Press).

Google Scholar

Furley, P., Dicks, One thousand., and Memmert, D. (2012). Nonverbal behavior in soccer: the influence of dominant and submissive body language on the impression germination and expectancy of success of soccer players. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 34, 61–82.

PubMed Abstract | Total Text | Google Scholar

Gage, J. (1993). Color and Culture: Practice and Significant from Artifact to Abstraction. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

Google Scholar

Garcia-Rubio, M. A., Picazo-Tadeo, A. J., and González-Gómez, F. (2011). Does a reddish shirt ameliorate sporting performance? Evidence from Castilian football. Appl. Econ. Lett. eighteen, 1001–1004. doi: 10.1080/13504851.2010.520666

CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar

Gegenfurtner, K. R., and Ennis, R. (in press). "Fundamentals of color vision Two: higher order color processing," in Handbook of Colour Psychology, eds A. Elliot, Thousand. Fairchild, and A. Franklin (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).

Google Scholar

Gnambs, T., Appel, Thou., and Batinic, B. (2010). Colour cherry-red in web-based knowledge testing. Comput. Hum. Behav. 26, 1625–1631. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2010.06.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goethe, Due west. (1810). Theory of Colors. London: Frank Cass.

Google Scholar

Goldstein, G. (1942). Some experimental observations concerning the influence of colors on the role of the organism. Occup. Ther. Rehab. 21, 147–151. doi: 10.1097/00002060-194206000-00002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenlees, I. A., Eynon, M., and Thelwell, R. C. (2013). Color of soccer goalkeepers' uniforms influences the outcomed of penalty kicks. Percept. Mot. Skill. 116, 1–10. doi: ten.2466/xxx.24.PMS.117x14z6

CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar

Greenlees, I., Leyland, A., Thelwell, R., and Filby, W. (2008). Soccer penalty takers' compatible colour and pre-penalty boot gaze affect the impressions formed of them by opposing goalkeepers. J. Sport Sci. 26, 569–576. doi: ten.1080/02640410701744446

PubMed Abstract | Total Text | CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar

Higham, J. P., and Winters, S. (in press). "Colour and mate choice in non-man animals," in Handbook of Color Psychology, eds A. Elliot, M. Fairchild, and A. Franklin (Cambridge: Cambridge University Printing).

Google Scholar

Johnson, G. M. (in printing). "Color appearance phenomena and visual illusions," in Handbook of Color Psychology, eds A. Elliot, Chiliad. Fairchild, and A. Franklin (Cambridge: Cambridge Academy Press).

Google Scholar

Kareklas, I., Brunel, F. F., and Coulter, R. A. (2014). Judgment is not color blind: the affect of automatic colour preference on product advertising preferences. J. Consum. Psychol. 24, 87–95. doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2013.09.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krenn, B. (2014). The impact of compatible color on judging tackles in association football. Psychol. Sport Exerc. fifteen, 222–225. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2013.11.007

CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar

Kuehni, R. (2012). Color: An Introduction to Do and Principles, 3rd Edn. New York, NY: Wiley. doi: 10.1002/9781118533567

CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar

Labrecque, L. L., and Milne, G. R. (2012). Exciting red and competent blue: the importance of color in marketing. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. forty, 711–727. doi: ten.1007/s11747-010-0245-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lakoff, G., and Johnson, Thousand. (1999). Philosophy in the Flesh: The Embodied Mind and its Challenges to Western Thought. New York, NY: Bones Books.

Google Scholar

Lee, South., and Rao, V. S. (2010). Color and shop option in electronic commerce: the explanatory role of trust. J. Electr. Commer. Res. eleven, 110–126.

Google Scholar

Lehrl, Southward., Gerstmeyer, M., Jacob, J. H., Frieling, H., Henkel, A. W., Meyrer, R., et al. (2007). Blue calorie-free improves cognitive performance. J. Neural Trans. 114, 457–460. doi: 10.1007/s00702-006-0621-four

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar

Levenson, R. Due west. (2003). Blood, sweat, and fears: the automatic architecture of emotion. Ann. N. Y. Acad Sci. 1000, 348–366. doi: 10.1196/annals.1280.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, H. (2014). Red-colored products heighten the attractiveness of women. Displays 35, 202–205. doi: ten.1016/j.displa.2014.05.009

CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar

Lindsay, D. T., Chocolate-brown, A. Grand., Reijnen, East., Rich, A. N., Kuzmova, Y. I., and Wolfe, J. M. (2010). Color channels, not color appearance of colour categories, guide visual search for desaturated color targets. Psychol. Sci. 21, 1208–1214. doi: x.1177/0956797610379861

PubMed Abstract | Total Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Little, A. C., and Hill, R. A. (2007). Attribution to blood-red suggests special office in authorisation signaling. J. Evol. Psychol. five, 161–168. doi: ten.1556/JEP.2007.1008

CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar

Lockley, S. W., Evans, E. East., Scheer, F. A., Brainard, Yard. C., Czeisler, C. A., and Aeschbach, D. (2006). Short-wavelength sensitivity for the direct furnishings of lite on alacrity, vigilance, and the waking electroencephalogram in humans. Sleep 29, 161–168.

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | Google Scholar

Lynn, One thousand., Giebelhausen, M., Garcia, Southward., Li, Y., and Patumanon, I. (in press). Habiliment colour and tipping: an attempted replication and extension. J. Hosp. Tourism Res. doi: x.1177/1096348013504001

CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar

Maier, Chiliad. A., Colina, R., Elliot, A. J., and Barton, R. A. (in press). "Color in accomplishment contexts in humans," in Handbook of Color Psychology, eds A. Elliot, M. Fairchild, and A. Franklin (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).

Google Scholar

Meier, B. P. (in press). "Do metaphors color our perception of social life?," in Handbook of Color sychology, eds A. Elliot, M. Fairchild, and A. Franklin (Cambridge: Cambridge Academy Press).

Google Scholar

Meier, B. P., and Robinson, M. D. (2005). The metaphorical representation of touch. Metaphor Symbol. xx, 239–257. doi: ten.1207/s15327868ms2004_1

CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar

O'Connor, Z. (2011). Color psychology and colour therapy: caveat emptor. Color Res. Appl. 36, 229–334. doi: 10.1002/col.20597

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pazda, A. D., and Greitemeyer, T. (in printing). "Color in romantic contexts in humans," in Handbook of Colour Psychology, eds A. Elliot, Thousand. Fairchild, and A. Franklin (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).

Google Scholar

Piqueras-Fiszman, B., Alcaide, J., Roura, E., and Spence, C. (2012). Is it the plate or is it the nutrient? Assessing the influence of the color (blackness or white) and shape of the plate on the perception of nutrient placed on it. Food Qual. Prefer. 24, 205–208. doi: ten.1016/j.foodqual.2011.08.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pomerleau, V. J., Fortier-Gauthier, U., Corriveau, I., Dell'Acqua, R., and Jolicœur, P. (2014). Colour-specific differences in attentional deployment for equiluminant popular-out colours: evidence from lateralized potentials. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 91, 194–205. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.10.016

PubMed Abstract | Total Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prado-León, 50. R., and Rosales-Cinco, R. A. (2011). "Effects of lightness and saturation on color associations in the Mexican population," in New Directions in Colour Studies, eds C. Biggam, C. Hough, C. Kay, and D. Simmons (Amsterdam, NL: John Benjamins Publishing Company), 389–394.

Google Scholar

Ridgway, J., and Myers, B. (2014). A study on brand personality: consumers' perceptions of colours used in style brand logos. Int. J. Fash. Des. Tech. Educ. seven, fifty–57. doi: ten.1080/17543266.2013.877987

CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, Due south. C., Owen, R. C., and Havlicek, J. (2010). Distinguishing between perceiver and wearer furnishings in clothing colour-associated attributions. Evol. Psychol. 8, 350–364.

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | Google Scholar

Ross, C. F., Bohlscheid, J., and Weller, K. (2008). Influence of visual masking technique on the assessment of 2 blood-red wines by trained consumer assessors. J. Food Sci. 73, S279–S285. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-3841.2008.00824.x

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar

Rutchick, A. K., Slepian, M. 50., and Ferris, B. D. (2010). The pen is mightier than the word: object priming of evaluative standards. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. twoscore, 704–708. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.753

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwarz, S., and Singer, M. (2013). Romantic red revisited: ruby enhances men's allure to young, just non menopausal women. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 49, 161–164. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2012.08.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Setchell, J. (in press). "Color in competition contexts in non-human animals," in Handbook of Color Psychology, eds A. Elliot, M. Fairchild, and A. Franklin (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).

Google Scholar

Sloane, P. (1991). Primary Sources, Selected Writings on Colour from Aristotle to Albers. New York, NY: Design Press.

Google Scholar

Smajic, A., Merritt, S., Banister, C., and Blinebry, A. (2014). The blood-red effect, feet, and exam performance: a multistudy examination. Teach. Psychol. 41, 37–43. doi: 10.1177/0098628313514176

CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar

Sokolik, Yard., Magee, R. G., and Ivory, J. D. (2014). Ruby-red-hot and ice-cold ads: the influence of spider web ads' warm and absurd colors on click-through ways. J. Interact. Advert. fourteen, 31–37. doi: 10.1080/15252019.2014.907757

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soldat, A. Southward., Sinclair, R. C., and Mark, Chiliad. Chiliad. (1997). Colour equally an ecology processing cue: external affective cues tin directly affect processing strategy without affecting mood. Soc. Cogn. 15, 55–71. doi: 10.1521/soco.1997.15.i.55

CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar

Sorokowski, P., and Szmajke, A. (2007). How does the "scarlet wins: event work? The part of sportswear color during sport competitions. Pol. J. Appl. Psychol. 5, 71–79.

Google Scholar

Sorokowski, P., Szmajke, A., Hamamura, T., Jiang, F., and Sorakowska, A. (2014). "Scarlet wins," "black wins," "bluish loses" effects are in the eye of the beholder, merely they are culturally niversal: a cross-cultural analysis of the influence of outfit colours on sports performance. Pol. Psychol. Bull. 45, 318–325. doi: 10.2478/ppb-2014-0039

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spence, C. (in printing). "Eating with our eyes," in Handbook of Color Psychology, eds A. Elliot, M. Fairchild, and A. Franklin (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).

Google Scholar

Spence, C., Velasco, C., and Knoeferle, G. (2014). A large sampled study on the influence of the multisensory surroundings on the vino drinking experience. Season 3, 8. doi: 10.1186/2044-7248-three-viii

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephen, I. D., Oldham, F. H., Perrett, D. I., and Barton, R. A. (2012a). Redness enhances perceived assailment, dominance and attractiveness in men'south faces. Evol. Psychol. 10, 562–572.

Google Scholar

Stephen, I. D., Scott, I. Grand. L., Coetzee, V., Pound, Due north., Perrett, D. I., and Penton-Voak, I. South. (2012b). Cross-cultural effects of color, only not morphological masculinity, on perceived attractiveness of men's faces. Evol. Hum. Behav. 33, 260–267. doi: ten.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.10.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephen, I. D., and Perrett, D. I. (in printing). "Color and face perception," in Handbook of Colour Psychology, eds A. Elliot, M. Fairchild, and A. Franklin (Cambridge: Cambridge University Printing).

Google Scholar

Stockman, A., and Brainard, D. H. (in press). "Fundamentals of colour vision I: processing in the eye," in Handbook of Colour Psychology, eds A. Elliot, M. Fairchild, and A. Franklin (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).

Google Scholar

Taillard, J., Capelli, A., Sagaspe, P., Anund, A., and Akerstadt, T. (2012). In-auto nocturnal blue calorie-free exposure improves motorway driving: a randomized controlled trial. PLoS ONE vii:e46750. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0046750

PubMed Abstract | Total Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanaka, A., and Tokuno, Y. (2011). The event of the color red on avoidance motivation. Soc. Behav. Pers. 39, 287–288. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2011.39.two.287

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thorstenson, C. A. (in press). Functional equivalence of the color cherry and enacted avoidance behavior? Replication and empirical integration. Soc. Psychol.

Google Scholar

Ten Velden, F. S., Baas, Chiliad., Shalvi, Due south., Preenen, P. T. Y., and De Dreu, C. Chiliad. W. (2012). In competitive interaction displays of carmine increase actors' competitive approach and perceivers' withdrawal. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 48, 1205–1208. doi: ten.1016/j.jesp.2012.04.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valdez, P., and Mehrabian, A. (1994). Effects of colour on emotions. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 123, 394–409. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.123.4.394

CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar

Van Ittersum, K., and Wansink, B. (2012). Plate size and color suggestability, The Deboeuf Illusion's bias on serving and eating behavior. J. Consum. Res. 39, 215–228. doi: 10.1086/662615

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandewalle, M., Schmidt, C., Albouy, G., Sterpenich, V., Darsaud, A., Rauchs, G., et al. (2007). Brain responses to violet, blueish, and green monochromatic low-cal exposures in humans: prominent role of blueish low-cal and the brainstem. PLoS Ane 11:e1247. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001247

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viola, A. U., James, L. Yard., Schlangen, L. J. Grand., and Dijk, D. J. (2008). Blue-enriched white lightin the workplace improves self-reported alertness, performance and sleep quality. Browse. J. Piece of work Environ. Health 34, 297–306. doi: ten.5271/sjweh.1268

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitfield, T. Westward., and Wiltshire, T. J. (1990). Colour psychology: a disquisitional review. Gen. Soc. Gen. Psychol. 116, 385–411.

Google Scholar

Yamazaki, A. K. (2010). An analysis of background-colour effects on the scores of a computer-based English language exam. KES Part Ii LNI. 6277, 630–636. doi: 10.1007/978-iii-642-15390-7_65

CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar

Young, S. (in press). The effect of ruddy on male perceptions of female attractiveness: moderationby baseline attractiveness of female faces. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol.

Google Scholar

Yüksel, A. (2009). Exterior color and perceived retail crowding: effects on tourists' shoppingquality inferences and approach behaviors. J. Qual. Assur. Hosp. Tourism 10, 233–254. doi: 10.1080/15280080903183383

CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar

Zanna, Chiliad. P., and Fazio, R. H. (1982). "The attitude beliefs relation: Moving toward a third generation of enquiry," in The Ontario Symposium, Vol. 2, eds Thousand. Zanna, E. T. Higgins, and C. Herman (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Assembly, Inc.), 283–301.

Google Scholar

bentonwoarearume.blogspot.com

Source: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00368/full

0 Response to "Color and Psychological Functioning a Review of Theoretical and Empirical Work"

Postar um comentário

Iklan Atas Artikel

Iklan Tengah Artikel 1

Iklan Tengah Artikel 2

Iklan Bawah Artikel